Ƶ

[Skip to Navigation]
Sign In
Figure 1. Inverse Kaplan-Meier Curves Depicting Cumulative Probability of Autism Spectrum Disorder, Overall and by Sex and Birth Year

Kaplan-Meier curves based on incidence at 0, 5, 10, 15, and 19 years of age by sex (A) and by birth year (B).

Figure 2. Possible Scenarios Under the Liability Threshold Model (LTM)

The LTM proposes that, for dichotomous traits (eg, ASD), there is an underlying continuous distribution of liability for that trait in a population, typically assumed to be Gaussian. If an individual’s liability (x-axis) exceeds a specific diagnostic threshold (represented by the black vertical solid line), the individual will be considered affected. The figures assume 1 single diagnostic threshold. Panel A illustrates the conventional assumption that males and females share the same liability distribution, with the same population mean and variance. In panel B, a female protective effect is illustrated, where males and females have the same distribution for ASD liability, but the male distribution is shifted toward the singular diagnostic threshold, resulting in more males than females exceeding the diagnostic threshold. Panel C shows the same mean for both sexes, suggesting no female protective effect (but instead males have a greater variance) resulting in more males than females exceeding the diagnostic threshold. In panel D, the liability distributions between males and females differ by their means and variances. The female mean is shifted toward the diagnostic threshold, but proportionally more males cross the liability threshold than females due to a greater variance. ASD indicates autism spectrum disorder.

Table 1. Characteristics of the Study Cohort
Table 2. Variance Components, Heritability Estimates, and Goodness-of-Fit Model for the Probability of Autism Spectrum Disordera
1.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years — autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, united states, 2010. Updated March 28, 2014. Accessed August 16, 2018.
2.
Newschaffer CJ, Fallin D, Lee NL. Heritable and nonheritable risk factors for autism spectrum disorders. Epidemiol Rev. 2002;24(2):137-153. doi:
3.
Klei L, McClain LL, Mahjani B, et al. How rare and common risk variation jointly affect liability for autism spectrum disorder. Mol Autism. 2021;12(1):66. doi:
4.
Ozonoff S, Young GS, Carter A, et al. Recurrence risk for autism spectrum disorders: a Baby Siblings Research Consortium study. ʱ徱ٰ. 2011;128(3):e488-e495. doi:
5.
Buxbaum JD, Hof PR. The Neuroscience of Autism Spectrum Disorders. Academic Press; 2013.
6.
Wing L. Sex ratios in early childhood autism and related conditions. Psychiatry Res. 1981;5(2):129-137. doi:
7.
Yang J, Zeng J, Goddard ME, Wray NR, Visscher PM. Concepts, estimation and interpretation of SNP-based heritability. Nat Genet. 2017;49(9):1304-1310. doi:
8.
Mitra I, Tsang K, Ladd-Acosta C, et al. Pleiotropic mechanisms indicated for sex differences in autism. PLoS Genet. 2016;12(11):e1006425. doi:
9.
Pettersson E, Lichtenstein P, Larsson H, et al; Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Working Group of the iPSYCH-Broad-PGC Consortium, Autism Spectrum Disorder Working Group of the iPSYCH-Broad-PGC Consortium, Bipolar Disorder Working Group of the PGC, Eating Disorder Working Group of the PGC, Major Depressive Disorder Working Group of the PGC, Obsessive Compulsive Disorders and Tourette Syndrome Working Group of the PGC, Schizophrenia CLOZUK, Substance Use Disorder Working Group of the PGC. Genetic influences on eight psychiatric disorders based on family data of 4 408 646 full and half-siblings, and genetic data of 333 748 cases and controls - CORRIGENDUM. Psychol Med. 2019;49(2):351-351. doi:
10.
Holmboe K, Rijsdijk FV, Hallett V, Happé F, Plomin R, Ronald A. Strong genetic influences on the stability of autistic traits in childhood. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;53(2):221-230. doi:
11.
Mayhew AJ, Meyre D. Assessing the heritability of complex traits in humans: methodological challenges and opportunities. Curr Genomics. 2017;18(4):332-340. doi:
12.
Axelsson O. The Swedish medical birth register. Acta Obstet Gynecol Scand. 2003;82(6):491-492. doi:
13.
Cnattingius S, Ericson A, Gunnarskog J, Källén B. A quality study of a medical birth registry. Scand J Soc Med. 1990;18(2):143-148. doi:
14.
Ekbom A. The Swedish multi-generation register. Methods Mol Biol. 2011;675:215-220. doi:
15.
Ronald A, Hoekstra RA. Autism spectrum disorders and autistic traits: a decade of new twin studies. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2011;156B(3):255-274. doi:
16.
Bai D, Yip BHK, Windham GC, et al. Association of genetic and environmental factors with autism in a 5-country cohort. Ѵ Psychiatry. 2019;76(10):1035-1043. doi:
17.
Ludvigsson JF, Andersson E, Ekbom A, et al. External review and validation of the Swedish national inpatient register. BMC Public Health. 2011;11:450. doi:
18.
Byrne N, Regan C, Howard L. Administrative registers in psychiatric research: a systematic review of validity studies. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2005;112(6):409-414. doi:
19.
Sandin S, Schendel D, Magnusson P, et al. Autism risk associated with parental age and with increasing difference in age between the parents. Mol Psychiatry. 2016;21(5):693-700. doi:
20.
McGuffin P, Owen MJ, Gottesman II. Quantitative genetics. In: Psychiatric Genetics and Genomics. Oxford University Press; 2004:44-48.
21.
Pawitan Y. In All Likelihood: Statistical Modelling and Inference Using Likelihood. Oxford University Press; 2001. doi:
22.
Christoffersen B, Mahjani B, Clements M, Kjellström H, Humphreys K. Quasi-monte carlo methods for binary event models with complex family data. J Comput Graph Stat. 2022;32(4):1383-1401.
23.
Iossifov I, O’Roak BJ, Sanders SJ, et al. The contribution of de novo coding mutations to autism spectrum disorder. ٳܰ. 2014;515(7526):216-221. doi:
24.
Gratten J, Wray NR, Peyrot WJ, McGrath JJ, Visscher PM, Goddard ME. Risk of psychiatric illness from advanced paternal age is not predominantly from de novo mutations. Nat Genet. 2016;48(7):718-724. doi:
25.
Christoffersen B. pedmod: pedigree models. cran.r-project. September 11, 2022. Accessed September 13, 2022.
26.
Sandin S, Lichtenstein P, Kuja-Halkola R, Hultman C, Larsson H, Reichenberg A. The heritability of autism spectrum disorder. Ѵ. 2017;318(12):1182-1184. doi:
27.
Williams JHG, Ross L. Consequences of prenatal toxin exposure for mental health in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2007;16(4):243-253. doi:
28.
DeFranco E, Moravec W, Xu F, et al. Exposure to airborne particulate matter during pregnancy is associated with preterm birth: a population-based cohort study. Environ Health. 2016;15:6. doi:
29.
Persson M, Reichenberg A, Andersson Franko M, Sandin S. Maternal type 1 diabetes, pre-term birth and risk of autism spectrum disorder-a prospective cohort study. Int J Epidemiol. 2023;52(2):377-385. doi:
30.
Hull L, Petrides KV, Mandy W. The female autism phenotype and camouflaging: a narrative review. Rev J Autism Dev Disord. 2020;7(4):306-317. doi:
31.
Reich R, Cloninger CR, Guze SB. The multifactorial model of disease transmission: I. Description of the model and its use in psychiatry. Br J Psychiatry. 1975;127:1-10. doi:
32.
Robinson EB, Lichtenstein P, Anckarsäter H, Happé F, Ronald A. Examining and interpreting the female protective effect against autistic behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110(13):5258-5262. doi:
33.
Tsai LY, Beisler JM. The development of sex differences in infantile autism. Br J Psychiatry. 1983;142:373-378. doi:
34.
Dougherty JD, Marrus N, Maloney SE, et al. Can the “female protective effect” liability threshold model explain sex differences in autism spectrum disorder? ܰDz. 2022;110(20):3243-3262. doi:
35.
Levy D, Ronemus M, Yamrom B, et al. Rare de novo and transmitted copy-number variation in autistic spectrum disorders. ܰDz. 2011;70(5):886-897. doi:
36.
Jacquemont S, Coe BP, Hersch M, et al. A higher mutational burden in females supports a “female protective model” in neurodevelopmental disorders. Am J Hum Genet. 2014;94(3):415-425. doi:
37.
Neale BM, Kou Y, Liu L, et al. Patterns and rates of exonic de novo mutations in autism spectrum disorders. ٳܰ. 2012;485(7397):242-245. doi:
38.
Doan RN, Lim ET, De Rubeis S, et al; Autism Sequencing Consortium. Recessive gene disruptions in autism spectrum disorder. Nat Genet. 2019;51(7):1092-1098. doi:
39.
Wigdor EM, Weiner DJ, Grove J, et al; iPSYCH Consortium. The female protective effect against autism spectrum disorder. Cell Genom. 2022;2(6):100134. doi:
40.
Bai D, Marrus N, Yip BHK, Reichenberg A, Constantino JN, Sandin S. Inherited risk for autism through maternal and paternal lineage. Biol Psychiatry. 2020;88(6):480-487. doi:
41.
Ben-Itzchak E, Ben-Shachar S, Zachor DA. Specific neurological phenotypes in autism spectrum disorders are associated with sex representation. Autism Res. 2013;6(6):596-604. doi:
42.
World Economic Forum. The global gender gap report 2015. 2015. Accessed October 5, 2022.
43.
Cnattingius S, Reilly M, Pawitan Y, Lichtenstein P. Maternal and fetal genetic factors account for most of familial aggregation of preeclampsia: a population-based Swedish cohort study. Am J Med Genet A. 2004;130A(4):365-371. doi:
44.
Tick B, Bolton P, Happé F, Rutter M, Rijsdijk F. Heritability of autism spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis of twin studies. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2016;57(5):585-595. doi:
45.
World Medical Association. World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. Ѵ. 2013;310(20):2191-2194. doi:
1 Comment for this article
EXPAND ALL
Misdiagnosis Epidemic; Autism masquerades as Depression, Anxiety and Hormones
Tricia Withey-Tebby |
I do not feel that reducing the diagnostic differences between the genders to a small section at the end of the paper is enough to highlight how much autism diagnosis difficulties confound the results. This is a retrospective cohort, meaning you cannot control the biases, especially in a subjective diagnosis such as the Autism Spectrum. It's not until recently that the diagnostic criteria took into account the differences in presentation between the genders. Studies have shown that 50% of boys with autism were diagnosed by 11, but only 20% of girls - that is not by any means an insignificant difference.

Studies have also shown that diagnostic bias requires girls to display more autistic traits than boys, especially since girls are more likely to be able to mask and present in other ways, especially when combined with other comorbid conditions, such as ADHD. In the 2013 DSM-5 it states “Autism spectrum disorder is diagnosed four times more often in males than in females. In clinic samples, females tend to be more likely to show accompanying intellectual disability, suggesting that girls without accompanying intellectual impairments or language delays may go unrecognized, perhaps because of subtler manifestation of social and communication difficulties.”

Could it not be a genetic heritability difference that causes the difference, but in fact a misdiagnosis epidemic that stems back decades and leads to women being told that they are depressed and anxious, or it's just "hormones", when in fact they are neurodiverse?

I am not a statistician, nor have I a medical background, but I do feel it is important to highlight clearly the struggles that are facing many people, and that it should be clearly noted that autism diagnosis has evolved markedly since the 90s - if a child was assessed as not autistic (under the guidelines of the day), they could quite possibly be autistic under current standards.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST: None Reported
READ MORE
Original Investigation
17, 2024

Examining Sex Differences in Autism Heritability

Author Affiliations
  • 1Department of Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Stockholm, Sweden
  • 2Department of Psychiatry, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, New York, New York
  • 3Seaver Autism Center for Research and Treatment at Mount Sinai, New York, New York
  • 4Jockey Club School of Public Health and Primary Care, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Hong Kong SAR
  • 5Institute for Human Genetics, University of California San Francisco
  • 6Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, University of California San Francisco
  • 7Weill Institute for Neurosciences, University of California San Francisco
  • 8Department of Genetics, Washington University School of Medicine, St Louis, Missouri
  • 9Department of Psychiatry, Washington University School of Medicine, St Louis, Missouri
  • 10Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities Research Center, Washington University School of Medicine, St Louis, Missouri
  • 11Pediatric Institute, Emory University School of Medicine, Atlanta, Georgia
  • 12Children’s Healthcare of Atlanta, Atlanta, Georgia
  • 13Department of Neurology, Washington University School of Medicine, St Louis, Missouri
  • 14Neurofibromatosis Center, Washington University School of Medicine, St Louis, Missouri
  • 15Institute of Developmental and Regenerative Medicine, Department of Paediatrics, University of Oxford, Oxford, United Kingdom
  • 16Department of Psychiatry and Behavioral Sciences, UCSF Weill Institute for Neurosciences, University of California, San Francisco
  • 17Churney ApS, Copenhagen, Denmark
JAMA Psychiatry. Published online April 17, 2024. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2024.0525
Key Points

Question What are the sex-specific etiological origins of autism spectrum disorder?

Findings In this cohort study including 1 047 649 Swedish children, 12 226 (1.17%) received a diagnosis of autism spectrum disorder; heritability was estimated at 87.0% for males and 75.7% for females, a statistically significant difference.

Meaning These findings suggest that variation in the occurrence of autism spectrum disorder in the population differs between males and females, indicating that some of the underlying causes and prevalence of the condition may differ between the 2 sexes.

Abstract

Importance Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder more prevalent in males than in females. The cause of ASD is largely genetic, but the association of genetics with the skewed sex ratio is not yet understood. To our knowledge, no large population-based study has provided estimates of heritability by sex.

Objective To estimate the sex-specific heritability of ASD.

Design, Setting, and Participants This was a population-based, retrospective analysis using national health registers of nontwin siblings and cousins from Sweden born between January 1, 1985, and December 31, 1998, with follow-up to 19 years of age. Data analysis occurred from August 2022 to November 2023.

Main Outcomes and Measures Models were fitted to estimate the relative variance in risk for ASD occurrence owing to sex-specific additive genetics, shared environmental effects, and a common residual term. The residual term conceptually captured other factors that promote individual behavioral variation (eg, maternal effects, de novo variants, rare genetic variants not additively inherited, or gene-environment interactions). Estimates were adjusted for differences in prevalence due to birth year and maternal and paternal age by sex.

Results The sample included 1 047 649 individuals in 456 832 families (538 283 males [51.38%]; 509 366 females [48.62%]). Within the entire sample, 12 226 (1.17%) received a diagnosis of ASD, comprising 8128 (1.51%) males and 4098 (0.80%) females. ASD heritability was estimated at 87.0% (95% CI, 81.4%-92.6%) for males and 75.7% (95% CI, 68.4%-83.1%) for females with a difference in heritability estimated at 11.3% (95% CI, 1.0%-21.6%). There was no support for shared environmental contributions.

Conclusions and Relevance These findings suggest that the degree of phenotypic variation attributable to genetic differences (heritability) differs between males and females, indicating that some of the underlying causes of the condition may differ between the 2 sexes. The skewed sex ratio in ASD may be partly explained by differences in genetic variance between the sexes.

Introduction

Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder present in 2% to 3% of children in the US.1 While the cause remains largely unknown, both common and rare genetic variants are associated with ASD.2,3 However, other factors are associated with trait variation, including sex.4,5 In order to understand the biological factors underlying ASD, it is important to elucidate the mechanisms underlying sex differences in ASD.2 It has been proposed that differences in prevalence between sexes is due to greater genetic variability in males (GVM).6

While a prior study using single-nucleotide variant (SNV) arrays7 showed similar SNV-based heritability in females and males,8 it should be noted that SNVs only capture a portion of the genetic variance.9 Thus, in contrast with the SNV-based study,7 a twin study10 using autistic traits as the outcome revealed higher heritability in males relative to females. However, as a result of the small sample size, the confidence intervals were wide and overlapping. More accurate estimates require population-based familial heritability analysis, which incorporates gene-gene interactions and familial aggregation.11 To address the heritability of ASD by sex, we leveraged a large population-based Swedish database to elucidate potential mechanisms underlying sex differences in ASD, calculating the narrow sense (ie, additive genetic) heritability using a sibling- and cousin-based study design.

Methods

This cohort study was approved by the national Swedish ethics review board and followed the Strengthening the Reporting of Observational Studies in Epidemiology () reporting guideline. No individual-level informed consent was required because all data used were anonymized and the study was conducted according to the Helsinki declaration.45

Population

The study included all live-born, singleton children from Swedish parents born between January 1, 1985, and December 31, 1998, identified from the Swedish Medical Birth Register. The register links the children with their mothers and has covered 99% of all births nationwide since 1973.12,13 Father identification was obtained from the Swedish Multi-Generation Register,14 which includes all Swedish citizens aged 15 years or younger and their parents since 1947 (covering all individuals alive since 1961, when the register was computerized). See eAppendix 1 and eFigure 1 in Supplement 1 for more information on how individuals were included in the study.

Analytical Data Set

To avoid biases due to differences in length of follow-up, and to optimize detection of ASD cases, we followed up all individuals up to age 19 years. Because twins are at increased risk of ASD15 and information about zygosity (necessary for heritability calculations in twins) was missing in our database, twins were excluded. To reduce genetic confounding and influences from missing values and parental identifiers, we included only children born to parents of Swedish origin. We further included only the first partner of the mother and father. We chose not to construct half-sibling families, which would rely on a questionable assumption about the correlation coefficient of the shared environmental effect. We restricted the analysis to the first 3 children of mothers in the parental and grandparental generations, further reducing the sample size, and specifically removed children where none of the grandparents were known.

Using a similar approach,16 we defined groups of dependent individuals as grandchildren of a set of grandparents yielding a pseudolikelihood where a child can be repeated at most twice. See eFigure 2, eFigure 3, eFigure 4, and eFigure 5 in Supplement 1 for examples of how offspring cousin pairs were identified.

ASD Diagnoses

In Sweden, all children undergo regular medical and developmental examinations. At age 4 years, a mandatory developmental assessment (motor, language, cognitive, and social) is conducted. Children with suspected developmental disorders are referred to a specialized team in a child psychiatry unit or habilitation service. Diagnoses from specialist care are reported to the Swedish National Patient Register (NPR).17 The NPR includes all ASD inpatient diagnoses since 1987 and outpatient visits since 2001. The diagnoses are coded using the International Classification of Diseases, Ninth Revision (ICD-9) and International Statistical Classification of Diseases and Related Health Problems, Tenth Revision (ICD-10). The NPR has been subjected to extensive validation efforts,17,18 including for autism diagnoses.19 Our database includes diagnoses in the NPR up to December 31, 2017, using ICD-9 code 299A from 1987 and ICD-10 code F84.0/1/3/5/8/9 from 1997.

Covariates

Several variables may bias the results. Because the ASD incidence has increased rapidly in the last 15 to 20 years (Figure 1), we include birth year from the Medical Birth Register. Similarly, parental age is associated with increased risk of ASD and was considered as a covariate. Death and emigration information from the Sweden Total Population Register and Migration helped to adjust for censoring in the survival analyses. See eAppendix 2 in Supplement 1 for more information on simulating bias for heritability differences.

Statistical Analysis

Kaplan-Meier depicts age-specific ASD incidence by 2 factors associated with ASD prevalence: sex and birth year. A 2-sided P <.05 was considered the threshold for statistical significance. Data analysis occurred from August 2022 to November 2023.

Sibling- and Cousin-Pair Correlations

Tetrachoric correlations were calculated to describe the overall pattern of genetic relatedness, an approach that relies on fewer but stronger assumptions regarding how the data were generated than required for the primary statistical models. A difference in correlation between male-male pairs and female-female pairs supports a sex difference in the genetic association with ASD risk. Tetrachoric correlation estimates the association of binary variables with underlying, normally distributed, continuous liabilities to develop a trait, which is not directly observable. Compared with the main analysis, this method does not utilize unpaired children or different-sex sibling pairs, which reduces the data by more than 50% and does not allow efficient adjustment for differences in prevalences (eg, between sexes and birth years).

Statistical Models

We modeled the probability of ASD using random-effect binary regression with a probit link, with fixed parameters to capture the liability thresholds and variance terms capturing the correlations due to inherited additive variance (Figure 2A and Figure 2B). The ASD status of an individual j in group i (ie, Yij) was assumed to be positive when the liability (ηij) passes 0. The liability was considered a function of both fixed and random effects (Yij = 1 when ηij ≥ 0 and Yij = 0 when ηij < 0), where ηij = xij′βĉ+ σAAij + σCCij +‰ӵij. The fixed parameters β captured differences in ASD prevalence. The xij values were assumed known fixed-effect design vectors, which, in our models, contained at most a fixed parameter for sex (male = 1 and female = 0), birth year (1985-1986, 1987-1989, 1990-1991, 1992-1994, and 1995-1998), and categories of paternal (≤28 years, 29-32 years, and ≥33 years) and maternal age (≤25 years, 26-30 years, and ≥31 years) at birth; and interactions between sex and birth year, paternal age, and maternal age. The categorical levels were based on quintiles for birth year and tertiles for parental age.

The random effects, Aij and Cij, were the additive genetic and shared environmental effects, respectively. A variance component, σA, captured the correlation due to additive genetic variants inherited directly from the parents. The Aij had a correlation of 0.5 for 2 full siblings, 0.125 for 2 cousins, and 0 otherwise. The component, σC, captured correlation from a shared childhood environment. The Cij had correlation of 1 for full siblings and 0 otherwise. Finally, the model included a residual random component (εij), capturing all other effects, uncorrelated for all individuals. We termed this as residual, instead of the commonly used nonshared environment, because the residual conceptually encompasses nonshared genetic factors as well.20 The random effects were assumed to follow a standard normal distribution. The scale parameters, σA and σC, were the primary focus in our analysis and gave the proportion of variance explained by each effect.

Sex-Specific Heritability Estimation

We were particularly interested in estimating the heritability by sex using the following equation:

σA2 / (1 + σA2 + σC2).

To achieve this outcome, we allowed the components σA and σC to differ for males and females (Figure 2C and D). We computed the 2-sided 95% CIs21 using recently developed estimation techniques that, unlike previously used methods including the tetrachoric correlations, allowed us to control for more variables and to use individual data without splitting the data by sex, resulting in greater statistical power and a substantially reduced computational time.22

First, we fitted a model (M) to allow for a comparison with earlier heritability studies in the same population considering sex as the only fixed parameter (M1). Next, M3 additionally adjusted for birth year and interaction between sex and birth year. M5 contained the variables in M3 along with paternal and maternal age at birth and sex interactions. Paternal age is associated with an increased rate of de novo ASD-associated variants.23,24 Because de novo variants would not be included in the inherited additive genetic effects, we adjusted for these. M2, M4, and M6 were similar to M1, M3, and M5, but with sex-specific scale parameters. The supplementary models used an alternative parametrization for parental age (M7) and adjusted for gestational age (M9) (eAppendix 3 in Supplement 1). M8 and M10 were their counterparts with sex-specific scale parameters.

The new and faster technique to approximate the likelihood, an extension of an earlier method,22 allowed us to estimate the extended model and include larger families and more control variables than in previous studies using the same computational time (see online appendix). Analysis data sets were created using SAS software version 9.41 (SAS Institute). The statistical liability models were calculated using the R statistical software version 4.0.5 on a Linux server with the package pedmod version 0.2.4 (R Project for Statistical Computing).25 See eAppendix 4 in Supplement 1 for information on the computer code used.

Exploratory Analyses: Assessing Alternative Scenarios

We assumed the female-specific residual (capturing a multitude of factors) was underlying the difference in heritability rather than the greater genetic variance in males. We also assumed there was a single binary factor that explains the difference in heritability (eAppendix 5 in Supplement 1).

Results

The analytical data set included a total of 1 047 649 individuals (eFigure 8 in Supplement 1) in 456 832 families (538 283 males [51.38%]; 509 366 females [48.62%]). By age 19 years, 12 226 individuals (1.17%) received an ASD diagnosis, including 8128 males (1.51%) and 4098 females (0.80%). The distribution of parental age and birth year was similar for males and females (Table 1). Sex- and birth year–specific prevalence are shown in the Kaplan-Meier plots, with higher cumulative ASD rates of diagnosis for males compared with females at age 19 years, as well as for later birth cohorts (1891 individuals [0.46%] for 19-year-olds born between 1985 and 1989 and 5327 individuals [2.36%] for 19-year-olds born between 1995 and 1998) (Figure 1).

Sibling- and Cousin-Pair Correlations

Within–sibling-pair correlation was estimated at 0.45 (95% CI, 0.40-0.51) for brother pairs and 0.37 (95% CI, 0.29-0.47) for sister pairs. For cousin pairs, corresponding correlations were estimated at 0.10 (95% CI, 0.05-0.17) and 0.12 (0.03-0.23) but with overlapping male and female confidence intervals for both siblings and cousins. Thus, in the presence of genetic component only, and assuming 50.0% shared genetic load for siblings and 12.5% shared genetic load for cousins, we would expect 90.0% (ie, 2 × 45.0%) vs 75.0% heritability estimated from male and female siblings and 81.6% vs 98.4% heritability from male and female cousins (eTable and eFigure 6 in Supplement 1). Broadly, these summary statistics suggest a 15.0% higher genetic male variance using full siblings only, but a 16.8% lower genetic variance and 98.4% female heritability using cousins only. However, these correlations provide imprecise estimates of differences in heritability and do not account for differences in prevalence between sexes and birth year or adjust for other potential confounders.

Heritability Estimates

Model parameters, variances, and goodness of fit are presented in Table 2 and a likelihood plot is exemplified in eFigure 7 in Supplement 1. For all models, the shared environmental effects were close to 0 and were not statistically significant. The overall heritability was estimated at 83.2% (95% CI, 79.3%-87.0%), after adjusting for birth year (M3). Adjusting for paternal and maternal age (ie, parameters potentially informative for increased variation due to de novo variants) resulted in a better model fit and a heritability estimate of 82.6% (95%CI, 78.7%-86.4%; M5). The conditional sex-specific heritability in M6 adjusting for birth year, sex and birth year interaction, and maternal and paternal age was estimated as 87.0% for males (95% CI, 81.4%-92.6%) and 75.7% for females (95% CI, 68.4%-83.1%), with a male vs female difference estimated at 11.3% (95% CI, 1.0%-21.6%). An alternative parametrization for parental age (M7 and M8) or adjustment for gestational age (M9 and M10) did not modify the estimates (Table 2).

Exploratory Analyses: Assessing Alternative Scenarios

Assuming that the female-specific residual is underlying the difference in heritability rather than greater genetic variance in males, the set of factors contributing to the female variance would have to be responsible for 37.0% of female ASD prevalence (eAppendix 5 in Supplement 1). If we instead assume there is a single binary factor that explains the difference in heritability, this factor must be very prevalent or very large. As a hypothetical example, if the sex difference in variance is due to a single unobserved factor among females, and it occurs in 6.0% of all females, it would have to increase the ASD risk by more than 10-fold. If it occurs in only 1.0% of females, it must increase the ASD risk by almost 40-fold (eFigure 8 in Supplement 1). Even with a prevalence as high as 6.0%, the factor would have to be sufficiently large to change the female ASD prevalence in a population from 0.5% to 5.5%.

Discussion

To our knowledge, this cohort study is the first large family- and population-based study estimating the sex-specific associations of genetic and noninherited factors with ASD liability. Compared with previous studies,16,26 we used newly developed statistical estimation techniques, which allow for a detailed inclusion of fixed parameters and provide better adjustments for differences in prevalence by sex and birth year. Using a nationwide sample from Sweden with clinically ascertained diagnoses of ASD, we demonstrated a modest, but statistically significant, difference in heritability between sexes. This male-female difference in heritability, adjusting for differences in ASD prevalence by sex, birth year, and maternal and paternal age, was estimated at 11.3% (95% CI, 1.0%-21.6%). Additional adjustment for gestational age, a primarily environmental factor, did not modify the estimates. In agreement with previous studies,16 there was no support for shared environmental contributions, and the narrow-sense heritability in the overall population was estimated at approximately 80.0%, with the remaining 20.0% explained by individual-specific effects.

Our results indicate that a relatively larger portion of ASD diagnoses can be explained by additive genetic sources in males relative to females. It is also possible that females are less impacted by additive genetic sources, or are particularly vulnerable to other risk sources.

These other risk sources (referred to as residual risk in our models rather than the potentially misleading environmental term20) could theoretically arise from (1) classic environmental factors found in the built environment, (2) differences in the cultural environment leading to ascertainment or diagnosis differences, (3) genetic sources not inherited additively (eg, de novo variants), or (4) deficits in the model or its assumptions, such as interactions among genetic risk factors or genes and environment (eg, toxins,27 pollution,28 and maternal effects like type 1 diabetes or rheumatoid arthritis29). Additional clinical behavioral factors should also be considered, such as female-typical autism presentation (ie, female autism phenotype)30 or de novo variants and variation from rare variants not inherited additively or gene-environmental interactions. These effects could be examined in fully genotyped samples (where de novo variant carriers could be excluded before calculating sex-specific heritability) or using biological models to understand the potential gene-environment interactions. However, our sensitivity analyses suggested that the residual factors alone are not likely to explain ASD prevalence differences between the sexes. In addition, 1 factor associated with the rate of de novo variants is parental age, both paternal and maternal,23 which we adjusted for in our model, with only minor differences in heritability estimates.

Previous heritability estimates are based on statistical models where equal variances for males and females are assumed, resulting in equal heritability, and where sex is included as a fixed factor to adjust for differences in prevalence (Figure 1B). Such models agree with the theory of female protective effect (FPE),31-34 where females require a higher genetic threshold to be diagnosed with ASD, resulting in a lower prevalence compared with males. However, existing research shows mixed support for the FPE model.11,34-40 One implication of the FPE model is that familial ASD liability would be expected to aggregate asymptomatically in sisters of affected probands, who would incur elevated rates of ASD in their offspring. However, a previous population-based study largely overlapping with the current study did not support this possibility.40

Interestingly, a model allowing for sex-specific heritability offers an alternative explanation of the difference in ASD prevalence.6,41 If males are more vulnerable to additive genetic risk, this model would be consistent with the GVM, and other models proposing an interaction between sex and genetic risk.34 The 2 theories of FPE and GVM are, however, not mutually exclusive and could coexist (Figure 2D).

Strengths and Limitations

This study has multiple distinguishing strengths. It includes data from a large, nationwide, population-based sample from a publicly financed and utilized health system ensuring study inclusion essentially free from selection biases. One may speculate that ASD data from Sweden, which is regarded as one of the most equal countries in the world42 may limit ascertainment biases due to sex. The extended 19-year follow-up minimized biases from differences in diagnosis age. Extending a previously published method25 to approximate the likelihood reduced the computing time, allowing the inclusion of larger families and more control variables to estimate variances by sex, without splitting the data set by sex.

Our study also has several limitations. Our results are based on individuals of Swedish origin only. In contrast with previous studies with greater restrictions,40,43,44 we only included the first 3 siblings or cousins born to each family. The approach of separating additive genetics, shared environment, and other contributions (ie, residual) relied on several untestable assumptions11 including that all individuals are ascertained independently. In addition, ASD diagnoses may be confounded by sex and temporal trends. This is possible given that the norms in diagnostic procedures and societal changes across time may have affected males and females differently, even though the sex ratio has remained stable.34 To address this, we included fixed parameters not only for sex and birth year, but also for their interactions. Additionally, there might be model misspecification. For example, we chose a probit link function over other possible link functions, whereas traditional liability threshold models rely on a Gaussian distribution. Also, both common and rare genetic variation are associated with ASD risk3; however, our study cannot determine whether common and rare variation equally contribute to sex differences in ASD risk. Additionally, we do not know to what extent the difference in estimated genetic variance can be attributed to phenotypic differences between males and females (eg, prevalence of ASD with and without comorbid conditions). Future studies could explore contributions of gene-environment interactions and underlying causes for estimated differences in heritability by incorporating ASD severity, cooccurring conditions, and age of first diagnosis to sex.

Conclusions

Based on population-based data from Sweden, this cohort study found that genetic variability in ASD liability differs between males and females, indicating that some of the underlying causes of the condition may differ between the 2 sexes. The skewed sex ratio in ASD may, partly, be explained by differences in genetic variance between sexes. This discovery opens up new avenues for further research aimed at gaining a deeper understanding of the prevalence of ASD.

Back to top
Article Information

Accepted for Publication: February 1, 2024.

Published Online: April 17, 2024. doi:10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2024.0525

Open Access: This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the CC-BY License. © 2024 Sandin S et al. JAMA Psychiatry.

Corresponding Author: Benjamin H.K. Yip, PhD, Jockey Club School of Public Health and Primary Care, The Chinese University of Hong Kong, Prince of Wales Hospital, Shatin, Hong Kong, China (benyip@cuhk.edu.hk); Sven Sandin, PhD, Department for Medical Epidemiology and Biostatistics, Karolinska Institutet, Nobels vag 12B, Stockholm 17177, Sweden (sven.sandin@ki.se).

Author Contributions: Dr Sandin had full access to all of the data in the study and takes responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.

Concept and design: Sandin, Yip, Weiss, Turner, Gutmann, Christoffersen.

Acquisition, analysis, or interpretation of data: Sandin, Yip, Yin, Dougherty, Fass, Constantino, Zhu, Turner, Marrus, Sanders, Christoffersen.

Drafting of the manuscript: Sandin, Yip, Dougherty, Gutmann, Christoffersen.

Critical review of the manuscript for important intellectual content: Sandin, Yip, Yin, Weiss, Dougherty, Fass, Constantino, Zhu, Turner, Marrus, Sanders, Christoffersen.

Statistical analysis: Sandin, Yin, Zhu, Christoffersen.

Obtained funding: Sandin, Yip, Weiss, Dougherty.

Administrative, technical, or material support: Sandin, Fass, Marrus.

Supervision: Sandin, Yip.

Conflict of Interest Disclosures: Dr Constantino reported receiving personal fees from Western Psychological Services and holding a patent with Western Psychological Services outside the submitted work. Dr Sanders reported receiving grants from BioMarin outside the submitted work. Dr Christofferson reported receiving personal fees from Mount Sinai outside the submitted work.

Funding/Support: The study was supported by the Simons foundation (PTE grant No 734069 to Drs Sandin, Dougherty, Fass, and Marrus), Swedish Research Council (grant No. Dnr 2021-0214 to Dr Yin), Karolinska Institutet Research Foundation (grant No Dnr 2020-02156) and Hong Kong General Research Fund of the University Grants Committee Research Grants Council (grant No. ECS24108520 to Dr Hailin).

Role of the Funder/Sponsor: The funders had no role in the design and conduct of the study; collection, management, analysis, and interpretation of the data; preparation, review, or approval of the manuscript; and decision to submit the manuscript for publication.

Data Sharing Statement: See Supplement 2.

References
1.
Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. Prevalence of autism spectrum disorder among children aged 8 years — autism and developmental disabilities monitoring network, 11 sites, united states, 2010. Updated March 28, 2014. Accessed August 16, 2018.
2.
Newschaffer CJ, Fallin D, Lee NL. Heritable and nonheritable risk factors for autism spectrum disorders. Epidemiol Rev. 2002;24(2):137-153. doi:
3.
Klei L, McClain LL, Mahjani B, et al. How rare and common risk variation jointly affect liability for autism spectrum disorder. Mol Autism. 2021;12(1):66. doi:
4.
Ozonoff S, Young GS, Carter A, et al. Recurrence risk for autism spectrum disorders: a Baby Siblings Research Consortium study. ʱ徱ٰ. 2011;128(3):e488-e495. doi:
5.
Buxbaum JD, Hof PR. The Neuroscience of Autism Spectrum Disorders. Academic Press; 2013.
6.
Wing L. Sex ratios in early childhood autism and related conditions. Psychiatry Res. 1981;5(2):129-137. doi:
7.
Yang J, Zeng J, Goddard ME, Wray NR, Visscher PM. Concepts, estimation and interpretation of SNP-based heritability. Nat Genet. 2017;49(9):1304-1310. doi:
8.
Mitra I, Tsang K, Ladd-Acosta C, et al. Pleiotropic mechanisms indicated for sex differences in autism. PLoS Genet. 2016;12(11):e1006425. doi:
9.
Pettersson E, Lichtenstein P, Larsson H, et al; Attention Deficit/Hyperactivity Disorder Working Group of the iPSYCH-Broad-PGC Consortium, Autism Spectrum Disorder Working Group of the iPSYCH-Broad-PGC Consortium, Bipolar Disorder Working Group of the PGC, Eating Disorder Working Group of the PGC, Major Depressive Disorder Working Group of the PGC, Obsessive Compulsive Disorders and Tourette Syndrome Working Group of the PGC, Schizophrenia CLOZUK, Substance Use Disorder Working Group of the PGC. Genetic influences on eight psychiatric disorders based on family data of 4 408 646 full and half-siblings, and genetic data of 333 748 cases and controls - CORRIGENDUM. Psychol Med. 2019;49(2):351-351. doi:
10.
Holmboe K, Rijsdijk FV, Hallett V, Happé F, Plomin R, Ronald A. Strong genetic influences on the stability of autistic traits in childhood. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2014;53(2):221-230. doi:
11.
Mayhew AJ, Meyre D. Assessing the heritability of complex traits in humans: methodological challenges and opportunities. Curr Genomics. 2017;18(4):332-340. doi:
12.
Axelsson O. The Swedish medical birth register. Acta Obstet Gynecol Scand. 2003;82(6):491-492. doi:
13.
Cnattingius S, Ericson A, Gunnarskog J, Källén B. A quality study of a medical birth registry. Scand J Soc Med. 1990;18(2):143-148. doi:
14.
Ekbom A. The Swedish multi-generation register. Methods Mol Biol. 2011;675:215-220. doi:
15.
Ronald A, Hoekstra RA. Autism spectrum disorders and autistic traits: a decade of new twin studies. Am J Med Genet B Neuropsychiatr Genet. 2011;156B(3):255-274. doi:
16.
Bai D, Yip BHK, Windham GC, et al. Association of genetic and environmental factors with autism in a 5-country cohort. Ѵ Psychiatry. 2019;76(10):1035-1043. doi:
17.
Ludvigsson JF, Andersson E, Ekbom A, et al. External review and validation of the Swedish national inpatient register. BMC Public Health. 2011;11:450. doi:
18.
Byrne N, Regan C, Howard L. Administrative registers in psychiatric research: a systematic review of validity studies. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2005;112(6):409-414. doi:
19.
Sandin S, Schendel D, Magnusson P, et al. Autism risk associated with parental age and with increasing difference in age between the parents. Mol Psychiatry. 2016;21(5):693-700. doi:
20.
McGuffin P, Owen MJ, Gottesman II. Quantitative genetics. In: Psychiatric Genetics and Genomics. Oxford University Press; 2004:44-48.
21.
Pawitan Y. In All Likelihood: Statistical Modelling and Inference Using Likelihood. Oxford University Press; 2001. doi:
22.
Christoffersen B, Mahjani B, Clements M, Kjellström H, Humphreys K. Quasi-monte carlo methods for binary event models with complex family data. J Comput Graph Stat. 2022;32(4):1383-1401.
23.
Iossifov I, O’Roak BJ, Sanders SJ, et al. The contribution of de novo coding mutations to autism spectrum disorder. ٳܰ. 2014;515(7526):216-221. doi:
24.
Gratten J, Wray NR, Peyrot WJ, McGrath JJ, Visscher PM, Goddard ME. Risk of psychiatric illness from advanced paternal age is not predominantly from de novo mutations. Nat Genet. 2016;48(7):718-724. doi:
25.
Christoffersen B. pedmod: pedigree models. cran.r-project. September 11, 2022. Accessed September 13, 2022.
26.
Sandin S, Lichtenstein P, Kuja-Halkola R, Hultman C, Larsson H, Reichenberg A. The heritability of autism spectrum disorder. Ѵ. 2017;318(12):1182-1184. doi:
27.
Williams JHG, Ross L. Consequences of prenatal toxin exposure for mental health in children and adolescents: a systematic review. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2007;16(4):243-253. doi:
28.
DeFranco E, Moravec W, Xu F, et al. Exposure to airborne particulate matter during pregnancy is associated with preterm birth: a population-based cohort study. Environ Health. 2016;15:6. doi:
29.
Persson M, Reichenberg A, Andersson Franko M, Sandin S. Maternal type 1 diabetes, pre-term birth and risk of autism spectrum disorder-a prospective cohort study. Int J Epidemiol. 2023;52(2):377-385. doi:
30.
Hull L, Petrides KV, Mandy W. The female autism phenotype and camouflaging: a narrative review. Rev J Autism Dev Disord. 2020;7(4):306-317. doi:
31.
Reich R, Cloninger CR, Guze SB. The multifactorial model of disease transmission: I. Description of the model and its use in psychiatry. Br J Psychiatry. 1975;127:1-10. doi:
32.
Robinson EB, Lichtenstein P, Anckarsäter H, Happé F, Ronald A. Examining and interpreting the female protective effect against autistic behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2013;110(13):5258-5262. doi:
33.
Tsai LY, Beisler JM. The development of sex differences in infantile autism. Br J Psychiatry. 1983;142:373-378. doi:
34.
Dougherty JD, Marrus N, Maloney SE, et al. Can the “female protective effect” liability threshold model explain sex differences in autism spectrum disorder? ܰDz. 2022;110(20):3243-3262. doi:
35.
Levy D, Ronemus M, Yamrom B, et al. Rare de novo and transmitted copy-number variation in autistic spectrum disorders. ܰDz. 2011;70(5):886-897. doi:
36.
Jacquemont S, Coe BP, Hersch M, et al. A higher mutational burden in females supports a “female protective model” in neurodevelopmental disorders. Am J Hum Genet. 2014;94(3):415-425. doi:
37.
Neale BM, Kou Y, Liu L, et al. Patterns and rates of exonic de novo mutations in autism spectrum disorders. ٳܰ. 2012;485(7397):242-245. doi:
38.
Doan RN, Lim ET, De Rubeis S, et al; Autism Sequencing Consortium. Recessive gene disruptions in autism spectrum disorder. Nat Genet. 2019;51(7):1092-1098. doi:
39.
Wigdor EM, Weiner DJ, Grove J, et al; iPSYCH Consortium. The female protective effect against autism spectrum disorder. Cell Genom. 2022;2(6):100134. doi:
40.
Bai D, Marrus N, Yip BHK, Reichenberg A, Constantino JN, Sandin S. Inherited risk for autism through maternal and paternal lineage. Biol Psychiatry. 2020;88(6):480-487. doi:
41.
Ben-Itzchak E, Ben-Shachar S, Zachor DA. Specific neurological phenotypes in autism spectrum disorders are associated with sex representation. Autism Res. 2013;6(6):596-604. doi:
42.
World Economic Forum. The global gender gap report 2015. 2015. Accessed October 5, 2022.
43.
Cnattingius S, Reilly M, Pawitan Y, Lichtenstein P. Maternal and fetal genetic factors account for most of familial aggregation of preeclampsia: a population-based Swedish cohort study. Am J Med Genet A. 2004;130A(4):365-371. doi:
44.
Tick B, Bolton P, Happé F, Rutter M, Rijsdijk F. Heritability of autism spectrum disorders: a meta-analysis of twin studies. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2016;57(5):585-595. doi:
45.
World Medical Association. World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. Ѵ. 2013;310(20):2191-2194. doi:
×